Is trypophobia real? (2024)

  • Journal List
  • BJPsych Open
  • v.10(2); 2024 Mar
  • PMC10897704

As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsem*nt of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice

Is trypophobia real? (1)

BJPsych Open. 2024 Mar; 10(2): e48.

Published online 2024 Feb 16. doi:10.1192/bjo.2023.621

PMCID: PMC10897704

PMID: 38362941

Geoff G. ColeIs trypophobia real? (2)*

Author information Article notes Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

Abstract

Approximately 10–18% of the adult population experience some form of anxiety when viewing clusters of small holes. ‘Trypophobia’ has been the subject of much discussion within the peer-reviewed literature, news outlets, health-related websites and social media. However, there is some scepticism surrounding the phenomenon. It is often stated that the condition is not recognised by the American Psychiatric Association, and not listed as a phobia in the DSM-5. It has also been claimed that trypophobia is no more than a particularly successful internet meme. In this editorial, I argue that such criticisms are misplaced. There is, for instance, no list of phobias in the DSM-5; only criteria that determine phobia classification. Using these criteria, as well as personal testimonials, trypophobia is clearly a phobia. Furthermore, the meme hypothesis cannot account for the fact that the phenomenon existed long before the internet.

Keywords: Anxiety or fear-related disorders, phobias, cognitive neuroscience, trypophobia, aetiology

In 2013, myself and Arnold Wilkins described a phenomenon in which individuals report aversion to clusters of small holes.1 Forty-seven peer-reviewed papers have now appeared on ‘trypophobia’, primarily concerned with the critical features of the stimuli that induce the condition and why it occurs (e.g. Yamada and Sasaki2). The paper also initiated much discussion on social media and generated hundreds of news articles (e.g. The Washington Post, CNN, Daily Mail, Independent, Huffington Post, BBC). The phenomenon has also been included in many medical and health-related websites. As Ng3 stated, trypophobia became a ‘buzzword online’. Thus, the narrative associated with the condition has not come solely from academia. However, many authors from both the peer- and non-peer-reviewed literature, have expressed scepticisms about the phenomenon. Here, I will rebut the central aspects of the discourse: that trypophobia is not a fear or phobia and is no more than a particularly successful internet meme.

Is trypophobia a fear?

The title of the Cole and Wilkins paper referred to trypophobia as a ‘fear’.1 This reflected the fact that the phenomenon had already been given the phobia title (online) and a phobia commonly means to have a ‘fear of’ something. Some authors (e.g. Can et al4) have taken this to mean that a fearful response is the central or typical reaction. Although the response of some individuals is best characterised as fear,5 most feel anxious as opposed to being scared.6 Indeed, trypophobia-inducing stimuli can cause a range of negative reactions, including disgust.7,8 The variety of possible emotions is the reason why Cole and Wilkins1 described trypophobia as an ‘aversion’ and suggested that, in addition to the psychiatric literature, it could be placed within the context of visual stress in which certain stimuli induce unpleasant effects, as with photosensitive epilepsy.

Trypophobia is evidently an aversion that induces some form of anxiety and discomfort that can be objectively measured (e.g. increased electrodermal activity9). Even the fact that trypophobia has been shown to induce disgust7,8 does not challenge the idea that phenomenon is an anxiety or aversion; spiders, well-known to induce aversion, also induce disgust.10 It is additionally worth noting that the DSM-511 does not separate fear from anxiety when outlining the seven diagnostic criteria for specific phobias; it instead refers to ‘fear or anxiety’ in all seven, and ‘avoidance’ in three. This is, of course, not to state that these responses are physiologically and/or psychologically the same; it simply acknowledges that the differences in fear, anxiety and avoidance may not always be particularly important when assessing phobia.

Trypophobia and the DSM-5

Many authors have also referred to the fact that trypophobia is not listed in the DSM-5. This has, for some, come to mean that the phenomenon is not formally ‘recognised’ by the American Psychiatric Association (APA).4,12 However, this argument misrepresents what the manual states with respect to phobia classification. There is no list of phobias, as there is no list of any other mental disorder. There are ‘codes’, presented in an appendix, which are list-like but, importantly, these are not used to determine whether a behaviour should be considered a phobia. Instead, the DSM-5 presents a series of criteria. It is these that are central to determining whether an aversion reaches the definition of a phobia, not any list. When a phobia is mentioned it is simply provided as an example. Indeed, very few phobias are mentioned but are nonetheless recognised as real phobias. For example, an aversion to cats does not appear anywhere, yet no one would argue that ailurophobia is ‘not recognised’ by the APA.

Importantly, trypophobia can be defined as a specific phobia when using all seven criteria outlined in the DSM-5.

The first criterion is that the aversion occurs in response to a ‘specific object or situation’. Indeed, the inducing stimuli are so specific that they possess relatively greater contrasts at different spatial frequencies compared with non-trypophobic holes.13,14 The second criterion is that the threat from the stimuli (e.g. aerated chocolate) is clearly ‘out of proportion to the actual danger posed’. The third criterion is satisfied as, given that trypophobia is induced by a visual stimulus rather than a situation, the response is always ‘immediate’. For the fourth criterion, the many testimonials given on the trypophobia Facebook discussion forum (Trypophobia: Fear of Clusters of Holes; see https://www.facebook.com/groups/3318322299) clearly show that the inducing images are ‘actively avoided’. Indeed, one of the moderating rules of this forum is that trypophobic images must not be posted. Such active avoidance is also supported by the peer-reviewed literature; Robakis5 reported that one trypophobic individual was not able to drive because of the light-emitting diode clusters in traffic lights. These testimonials also show that the condition fulfils the fifth criterion of ‘typically lasting for 6 months or more’, with sufferers often stating that their trypophobia began in childhood. Furthermore, in a survey of 195 users of the trypophobia Facebook discussion forum, Vlok-Barnard and Stein6 found that the condition had a mean duration of 18 years and a range that went up to 60 years. The sixth criterion in the DSM-5 also states that the phobia must not be ‘better explained by the symptoms of another mental disorder’ that also include fear, anxiety and avoidance. Four peer-reviewed article have now shown that trypophobia is weakly,1 if at all, associated with general anxiety15,16 and general mental health outcomes.17 Finally, for the seventh criterion, trypophobic images can create ‘significant distress or impairment in social, occupational, or other important areas of functioning’. The Vlok-Barnard and Stein survey also showed that 15.4% of respondents scored at least five on the work or school subscale of the Sheehan Disability Scale (SDS).18 Such a score classifies an individual as having ‘significant impairment’ in the relevant activity. With specific reference to ‘distress’, the survey additionally found that 25.8% of respondents reported ‘a high level of psychological distress’ and a further 24.7% reported ‘a very high level’.

This last DSM-5 criterion also shows how comparable trypophobia is to other specific phobias. For example, in a worldwide sample of over 7000 participants with specific phobia, Wardenaar et al19 found that only 10.3% scored at least seven (‘severe impairment’) on the work subscale of the SDS. As Becker et al20 noted, ‘Specific phobias are often considered less impairing than other disorders, since the feared object or situation is circ*mscribed and therefore its avoidance is much easier than, for example, in social phobia or agoraphobia’.

Trypophobia also reaches the definition of a specific phobia as outlined by the alternative ICD-11,21 published by the World Health Organization. This is to be expected, given the large overlap of the two instruments with respect to the criteria for a specific phobia. Six ‘essential’ or ‘required features’ are described. The ‘fear, anxiety, or avoidance’: consistently occurs upon exposure, is out of proportion to the actual danger posed, lasts for at least several months, is not better accounted for by another mental disorder, impairs functioning in a range of situations (e.g. educational, occupational) and is actively avoided. Note that the ICD-11 also allows for odd or unusual phobias, which could include trypophobia.

Is trypophobia no more than a successful internet meme?

The issue of ‘recognition’ is related to perhaps the most serious criticism: that trypophobia does not exist at all. For example, Can et al4 suggest that individuals only ‘claim’ to have trypophobia, and Smith,22 writing in Medical News Today, asks ‘Is trypophobia real?’. To challenge the notion that people report negative reactions to trypophobic stimuli is an odd position to take. Scepticism of the basic phenomenon is also reflected in the phrase that a person can ‘have a phobia to anything’.23 Although classical conditioning can result in phobia to any stimulus, opponents have to explain why 10–18% of the adult population17,24 are averse to a very diverse range of trypophobic stimuli that all have a particular spectral property in common: relatively high contrasts at a range of spatial frequencies, which is a property that individuals would not be conscious of. Thus, it is difficult (for opponents) to explain why manipulations in luminance contrast14 and phase spectrum influence responses.15 This cannot be easily explained through conditioning. Furthermore, trypophobic stimuli have been shown to induce other very specific effects. For example, they subtly influence eye movement trajectories in a way that does not occur with control images.25

Although the present author rejects criticisms concerning response (i.e. fear, response or avoidance) and DSM-5 classification, it is possible that some people have ‘caught’ trypophobia via its ubiquitous online presence, or as Oelze26 states, ‘a fear made worse by the internet’. It is certainly the case that many people first came across trypophobia via a number of widely circulated images in which holes were digitally incorporated onto human skin. However, even if the internet meme theory is true this would be no different to many other phobias. That is, a condition passed on via social influence in which a person becomes aware of an aversion experienced by, for example, a family member. There is also the acquisition of knowledge concerning societal representation and view of certain objects (e.g. snakes, co*ckroaches). The present author will also note that many of the 100 or so individuals with trypophobia he has had contact with in the past decade stated that they remember having the condition before the internet existed. Typically, these individuals have been aged ≥50 years and recall first seeing the inducing stimuli as children in the 1970s and 1980s. This clearly refutes the notion that trypophobia is solely an internet-induced phenomenon.

Future work and conclusions

A major challenge is to identify aetiology. In contrast to the social learning theories are evolutionary accounts. For instance, the spectral composition of trypophobic stimuli, in terms of contrasts and spatial frequency, is particularly unique and shared with the aposematic patterns typically present on the skin of poisonous animals.1 Humans may therefore have evolved to be particularly sensitive to this feature. The condition may also be related to sensitivity toward skin disease and pathology, which often present as clusters of circles.2,7 Indeed, incorporating hole clusters onto (images of) human skin significantly increases the degree to which the images are judged to be averse.16 There is also the issue of how reactions to trypophobia-inducing stimuli are best characterised. Evidence so far suggests that the most common response is itchiness, goosebumps and nausea.6 Disgust is also common.7,8 Future large-scale studies also need to determine specific prevalence. The largest study conducted so far, by Cole et al24 (N=2558, 47% men, UK citizens, a range of socioeconomic backgrounds and educational history, relatively even distribution of participants across six age categories ranging from 18 to ≥65 years), shows that 9.7% of respondents reached the threshold for being defined as trypophobic, as determined by the commonly used Trypophobia Questionnaire.13 Furthermore, although 57% of respondents indicated that they felt no anxiety at all when viewing the two trypophobic images presented as part of the scale, 33% responded ‘slightly’ or ‘moderately’ on at least one of the 17 items that asks observers to consider the degree to which a statement is appropriate to them (e.g. ‘Feel aversion, disgust or repulsion’, ‘Have trouble breathing’). This suggests that trypophobia is a matter of degree that can include a milder form. Although the condition satisfies the criteria for being clinically significant (e.g. persists for at least 6 months), this study did not determine whether positive participants met the criteria for specific phobia. Thus, some individuals may experience aversion to hole clusters without meeting the DSM-5 criteria. A further large-scale (N=2065) assessment of trypophobia places the prevalence figure at 17.6%.17 However, this was conducted on individuals aged 15–24 years, and it is known that instances of phobia decreases as age increases,27 including trypophobia.24 Taken together,17,24 prevalence of trypophobia is between 10 and 18%. This fits well with the first estimate of around 16%.1 It is also important for future studies to determine the comorbidities associated with trypophobia. For example, Wong et al17 noted that, ‘As young people are often less likely to seek professional help for mental health needs, incorporating measures of trypophobia in future routine screening may improve both youth engagement and possible earlier detection of young people at greater psychiatric risk’. In this context, Wong et al found that symptoms of depression and trypophobia were significantly, albeit weakly, associated. Finally, there is the issue of potential treatments. Although there is not yet firm evidence on this issue, it may be the case that interventions known to help with the treatment of other specific phobias will be effective.

In sum, clusters of holes induce various forms of anxiety that can be measured both psychologically and physiologically.28 As with most other phobias, the phenomenon is not mentioned or ‘listed’ in the DSM-5. Trypophobia does, however, reach the definition of a specific phobia; a phobia that, furthermore, is not uncommon. Indeed, the present author is currently examining the possibility that everyone is sensitive to trypophobic stimuli – although they may not feel anxious, most people can appreciate that there is something not quite right about the lotus seed pod (see Fig. 1).

Open in a separate window

Fig. 1

Image of a lotus seed pod.

Biography

Is trypophobia real? (4)Geoff G. Cole, PhD, is a senior lecturer at the Centre for Brain Science, University of Essex. He specialises in visual cognition.

Funding

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Declaration of interest

Although the phenomenon was previously known, trypophobia was first described in the peer-reviewed literature by the author.

References

1. Cole GG, Wilkins A. The fear of holes. Psychol Sci2013; 24: 1980–5. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

2. Yamada Y, Sasaki K. Involuntary protection against dermatosis: a preliminary observation on trypophobia. BMC Res Notes2017; 10: 658. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

3. Ng C. Trypophobia, the Fear of Holes, May Not Be a Real Phobia: study. The Straits Times, 2018. (https://www.straitstimes.com/world/united-states/trypophobia-the-fear-of-holes-may-not-be-a-real-phobia-study). [Google Scholar]

4. Can W, Zhuoran Z, Zheng J. Is trypophobia a phobia?Psychol Rep2017; 120: 206–18. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

5. Robakis TK. Trypophobia associated with gabapentin a case report. J Clin Psychopharmacol2018; 38: 2. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

6. Vlok-Barnard M, Stein DJ. Trypophobia: an investigation of clinical features. Braz J Psychiatry2017; 39: 337–41. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

7. Kupfer TR, Le AT. Disgusting clusters: trypophobia as an overgeneralised disease avoidance response. Cogn Emot2017; 32: 729–41. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

8. Imaizumi S, Furuno M, Hibino H, Koyama S. Trypophobia is predicted by disgust sensitivity, empathic traits, and visual discomfort. SpringerPlus2016; 5: 1449. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

9. Pipitone RN, Gallegos B, Walters D. Physiological responses to trypophobic images and further scale validity of the Trypophobia Questionnaire. Pers Individ Diff2017; 108: 66–8. [Google Scholar]

10. Mulkens SAN, de Jong PJ, Merckelbach H. Disgust and spider phobia. J Abnorm Psychol1996; 105: 464–8. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

11. American Psychiatric Association. Anxiety disorders. In Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders 5th ed. American Psychiatric Association, 2013. [Google Scholar]

12. Doucleff M. Fear of Cantaloupes and Crumpets? A ‘Phobia’ Rises from the Web. NPR Magazine, 2013. (https://www.npr.org/sections/thesalt/2013/02/13/171383429/fear-of-cantalopes-and-crumpets-a-phobia-risesfrom-the-web). [Google Scholar]

13. Le A, Cole GG, Wilkins A. Assessment of trypophobia and an analysis of its visual precipitation. Q J Exp Psychol2015; 68: 2304–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

14. Sasaki K, Yamada Y, Kuroki D, Miura K. Trypophobic discomfort is spatial-frequency dependent. Adv Cogn Psychol2017; 13: 224–31. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

15. Pipitone RN, DiMattina C. Object clusters or spectral energy? Assessing the relative contributions of image phase and amplitude spectra to trypophobia. Front Psychol2020; 11: 1847. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

16. Pipitone RN, DiMattina C, Martin E, Pavela Banai I, Bellmore K, De Angelis M. Evaluating the ‘skin disease-avoidance’ and ‘dangerous animal’ frameworks for understanding trypophobia. Cogn Emot2022; 36: 943–56. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

17. Wong SM, Tang EY, Hui CL, Suen YN, Chan SK, Lee EH, et al. Excessive fear of clusters of holes, its interaction with stressful life events and the association with anxiety and depressive symptoms: large epidemiological study of young people in Hong Kong. BJPsych Open2023; 9(5): e151. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

18. Sheehan DV. Sheehan Disability Scale. In Handbook of Psychiatric Measures: 113–5. American Psychiatric Association, 2000. [Google Scholar]

19. Wardenaar KJ, Lim CC, Al-Hamzawi AO, Alonso J, Andrade LH, Benjet CD, et al. The cross-national epidemiology of specific phobia in the world mental health surveys. Psychol Med2017; 47: 1744–60. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

20. Becker ES, Rinck M, Türke V, Kause P, Goodwin R, Neumer S, et al. Epidemiology of specific phobia subtypes: findings from the Dresden mental health study. Eur Psychiatry 2007; 22: 69–74. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

21. World Health Organization (WHO). International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems 11th ed., ICD-11. WHO, 2021. [Google Scholar]

22. Smith L. Is Trypophobia Real?. Medical News Today, 2018. (https://www.medicalnewstoday.com/articles/320512).

23. Mathews C. In Doucleff M. Fear of Cantaloupes and Crumpets? A ‘Phobia’ Rises from the Web. NPR Magazine, 2013. (https://www.npr.org/sections/thesalt/2013/02/13/171383429/fear-of-cantalopes-and-crumpets-a-phobia-risesfrom-the-web).

24. Cole GG, Millett A, Juanchich M. The social influence theory of trypophobia. Q J Exp Psychol [Epub ahead of print] 4 Feb 2024. Available from: 10.1177/17470218241232665. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

25. Shirai R, Banno H, Ogawa H. Trypophobic images induce oculomotor capture and inhibition. Atten Percept Psychophys2019; 81: 420–32. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

26. Oelze P. Trypophobia: a Fear Made Worse by the Internet. BetterHelp, 2018. (https://www.betterhelp.com/advice/phobias/trypophobia-a-fear-made-worse-by-the-internet/). [Google Scholar]

27. Fredrikson M, Annas P, Fischer H, Wik G. Gender and age differences in the prevalence of specific fears and phobias. Behav Res Ther1996; 34: 33–9. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

28. Mayor E, Meyer A, Miani A, Lieb R. An exploration of the nomological network of trypophobia. PLoS One2021; 16(9): e0257409. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from BJPsych Open are provided here courtesy of Royal College of Psychiatrists

Is trypophobia real? (2024)

FAQs

Is trypophobia real? ›

There is, for instance, no list of phobias in the DSM-5; only criteria that determine phobia classification. Using these criteria, as well as personal testimonials, trypophobia is clearly a phobia. Furthermore, the meme hypothesis cannot account for the fact that the phenomenon existed long before the internet.

Is trypophobia disease real? ›

Trypophobia, a relatively new term, is the fear of clustered holes, bumps, or nodules. However, trypophobia skin is not a real skin disease, nor is trypophobia a diagnosable mental disorder. Trypophobia is the fear of clustered holes, bumps, or nodules.

Why is trypophobia so disgusting? ›

Cole and Wilkins also stated that the level of disgust with trypophobia increases if the holes are on human skin. Writing in Popular Science, Jennifer Abbasi argues that emotional contagion within such social media groups may be responsible for some of the aversive reactions to such images.

Is trypophobia a natural thing? ›

According to one of the most popular theories, trypophobia is an evolutionary response to things that are associated with disease or danger. 7 Diseased skin, parasites, and other infectious conditions, for example, may be characterized by such holes or bumps.

Is trypophobia official? ›

The American Psychiatric Association doesn't officially recognize this disorder in its Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5), a large volume of all known mental illnesses and their symptoms. Trypophobia is more of an aversion or feeling of disgust, rather than fear.

Does trypophobia hurt? ›

Trypophobia is mainly visual. If you have this phobia, you might feel anxiety, disgust, and discomfort when looking at things like: lotus seed pods. honeycombs.

Who suffer from trypophobia? ›

Generally, women are more likely to suffer from trypophobia as compared to men. Your chances of showing trypophobia symptoms increase if you are currently suffering from depression, anxiety, OCD or obsessive-compulsive disorder.

Is it rare to have trypophobia? ›

Approximately 10–18% of the adult population experience some form of anxiety when viewing clusters of small holes. 'Trypophobia' has been the subject of much discussion within the peer-reviewed literature, news outlets, health-related websites and social media.

Why do holes bother me? ›

Trypophobia has been described as an excessive and persistent fear of clusters of holes. Spectral features of visual stimuli characterised by high-contrast energy at midrange spatial frequencies have been considered to underlie the condition.

What is megalophobia? ›

A person with megalophobia experiences intense fear and anxiety when they think of or are around large objects such as large buildings, statues, animals and vehicles. They often avoid situations or places that have large objects.

What is thalassophobia? ›

Thalassophobia is a fear of the ocean or other large bodies of water. It may stem from a traumatic childhood event, which a person may have experienced directly, seen, possibly onscreen, or heard about. Several types of therapy, including CBT and exposure therapy, can help reduce the impact of phobias.

How do I know if I'm trypophobia? ›

Trypophobia is a specific type of phobia characterized by a fear of holes. People with trypophobia may express feelings of disgust or fear, as well as other nonspecific symptoms, when encountering stimuli containing irregular patterns or clustered holes.

Does trypophobia make you itch? ›

Around 10 to 15 percent of people find these images uncomfortable to look at, said Nate Pipitone, an associate professor of psychology at Florida Gulf Coast University, who has been studying trypophobia for several years. The images can cause feelings of repulsion and disgust as well as itching and nausea.

What is Hippopotomonstrosesquippedaliophobia the fear of? ›

Hippopotomonstrosesquippedaliophobia refers to the phobia or fear of long words. Feelings of shame or fear of ridicule for mispronouncing long words may cause distress or anxiety. Phobia isn't officially recognized as a diagnosis, so more research is needed.

Is trypophobia a survival instinct? ›

Thus, the discomfort felt toward trypophobic images might be an instinctive response to their visual characteristics rather than the result of a learned but nonconscious association with venomous animals.

What is scopophobia? ›

Scopophobia is an exaggerated fear of being looked at or watched. People with scopophobia typically feel highly self-conscious and often avoid social situations. Some don't even like to make eye contact with other people.

Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Gov. Deandrea McKenzie

Last Updated:

Views: 6596

Rating: 4.6 / 5 (66 voted)

Reviews: 81% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Gov. Deandrea McKenzie

Birthday: 2001-01-17

Address: Suite 769 2454 Marsha Coves, Debbieton, MS 95002

Phone: +813077629322

Job: Real-Estate Executive

Hobby: Archery, Metal detecting, Kitesurfing, Genealogy, Kitesurfing, Calligraphy, Roller skating

Introduction: My name is Gov. Deandrea McKenzie, I am a spotless, clean, glamorous, sparkling, adventurous, nice, brainy person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.